It doesn’t pay to be smart, at least for bumblebees, according to a new U of G study.
Prof. Nigel Raine has discovered that fast-learning bumblebees died sooner than their slower-learning co-workers.
He also found quick learners collected food only as fast as less smart bees in the colony and completed no more foraging bouts each day.
“Ultimately, the results revealed that fast-learning bumblebees collected fewer resources for the colony over their foraging career,” said Raine, holder of the Rebanks Family Chair in Pollinator Conservation. “These findings provide the first evidence of a learning-associated cost in the wild.”
This research was recently featured in the Globe and Mail, Toronto Star, CityNews, CTV and Yahoo News. It was also covered last week by the Daily Mail and the New Zealand Herald.
Published recently in Scientific Reports, the study highlights the potential cost of cleverness and could have implications for a variety of species beyond bees.
“Our results are surprising because we typically associate enhanced learning performance and cognitive ability with improved fitness, because it is considered beneficial to the survival of an individual or group,” said co-author Lisa Evans, with Plant and Food Research in New Zealand.
The researchers suggest that the energy demands of intelligence eat up limited resources, leaving smart bees with less energy for foraging than slower-learning counterparts.
“Neural tissue is metabolically expensive to produce and maintain,” said Evans. “Foraging is energy-demanding, but so is learning. This may explain the significantly shorter foraging lifespan of fast-learning bumblebees.”
In the lab, the researchers used blue and yellow artificial flowers to test the visual learning performance of 85 foraging bumblebees from five colonies.
The yellow flowers provided a sugar reward and the blue ones did not. The researchers observed how long it took the bees to ignore the unrewarding blue flowers, which are innately their preferred choice, and instead to associate yellow flowers with a food reward.
The bees were then monitored in the field using radio frequency identification tagging technology to determine their foraging activity and the quality of nectar or pollen they brought back to the nest.
“This finding is particularly interesting because we know that learning is really important for bees,” said Raine. “They learn which flowers provide the most rewards, when and where to find them – often in habitats containing dozens of flower species.”
So why has fast learning not been bred out of the bumblebee population? Raine said quick learning may or may not confer an advantage depending on the colony’s environment.
“In a more complex or changeable environment, these enhanced learning abilities may be vital to ensure colony resilience. Then it could pay to have some smart heads in the room.”
The findings may be useful for conserving habitat and pollinators that help in commercial crop production, he added.
“By determining how pollinators are adapted to their environment, we gain insight into the aspects of the environment that are important for colony success, which feeds into habitat and pollinator conservation.”